In the realm of social psychology, the enduring tension between individual morality and authority’s coercive power continues to captivate researchers and the public alike. A newly published study from Ohio State University revisits this dynamic through the lens of the infamous Milgram shock experiment, revealing profound insights about human self-perception and obedience under pressure. The study illuminates a prevalent cognitive bias known as the “better-than-average effect,” which leads individuals to overestimate their moral fortitude when faced with authoritative demands, thereby underestimating the extent to which social pressures influence compliance.
Originally conducted by Stanley Milgram in the 1960s, the shock experiment sought to probe the limits of obedience to authority. Participants were instructed by an experimenter to administer electric shocks to a learner, with the shocks increasing in severity and potential harm. Despite their moral objections, many complied fully, highlighting the powerful grip of authoritative command over personal ethics. Leveraging this foundational experiment, the Ohio State research team designed an imaginal replication study that did not subject participants to actual shocks but asked them to place themselves mentally in the Milgram scenario and predict their own and average individuals’ likely behavior.
The research, led by Associate Professor Philip Mazzocco and involving over 400 adult participants, presented first-person narratives of the original shock study. Participants were then asked to estimate at which point on a dial—from 1 (immediate cessation of shocks) to 31 (full obedience)—they would disobey the orders. The striking finding was that participants consistently predicted they would quit much earlier, around level 7, whereas they expected the average person to continue until around level 12. This discrepancy underscores a pervasive overconfidence in one’s resistance to unethical authority, a hallmark of the better-than-average bias in social cognition.
More intriguingly, exposing participants to empirical data from the original Milgram study—that 65% of participants exhibited complete obedience—altered their predictions for others but not for themselves. Those informed anticipated that the average person would persist longer in administering shocks, yet still believed they individually would demonstrate stronger moral resolve. This resistance to updating self-assessment in light of objective evidence points to a deep-rooted psychological defense mechanism aimed at preserving a positive self-image, even when confronted with contradictory facts.
This study also emphasizes the nuanced role imagination plays in ethical decision-making. Despite participants having detailed accounts to internalize the Milgram scenario, mere imagination was insufficient in activating the social pressures that typically compel obedience in real-life settings. Mazzocco analogizes this phenomenon to watching a horror movie from a safe place: the viewer intellectually understands the threat yet cannot fully experience the visceral fear that prompts survival behaviors. This distinction highlights the challenges in accurately forecasting one’s conduct in ethically compromising situations without feeling the genuine psychological stress involved.
The implications of these findings go beyond academic curiosity, touching upon how societies might foster resilience against coercive authorities intent on exploiting social compliance. If individuals collectively believe they are exceptions to the rule—that they alone can resist immoral commands—they may overlook the subtle but potent mechanisms by which authority can shape behavior. Educational and psychological interventions thus need to foster awareness of social influence dynamics and encourage strategies that acknowledge human susceptibility rather than deny it.
Personality traits further modulate obedience tendencies. Among the traits assessed in the study, conscientiousness emerged as a significant predictor: individuals exhibiting a strong sense of duty and adherence to rules were paradoxically more prone to complete the shock sequence, possibly out of a desire to fulfill perceived obligations to authority figures. This finding complicates simplistic narratives that equate conscientiousness with ethical inflexibility; instead, it suggests conscientiousness can sometimes drive compliance even when it conflicts with personal morals.
Prior knowledge of the Milgram experiment, surprisingly, did not readily translate into more accurate self-assessments. Around 65% of participants had not heard of the study before, and regardless of prior familiarity, estimates of personal resistance to obedience remained overly optimistic. This demonstrates the persistence of cognitive biases that shield self-perception from objective understanding, reinforcing the need for experiential or immersive learning modalities to better prepare individuals for real-world social pressures.
Beyond individual psychology, the study’s insights have profound ramifications for collective social behavior and institutional trust. In environments where authority is unchecked, or where there is a culture of unquestioning obedience, the risk that ordinary individuals will act contrary to their ethical standards is magnified. Awareness of the better-than-average effect can serve as a societal safeguard, prompting communities to develop systems that mitigate blind obedience and promote critical engagement with authority directives.
Mazzocco advocates for deliberate cultivation of curiosity as a means to bolster ethical resilience. By encouraging individuals to question and explore the motives behind authoritative commands, curiosity becomes a psychological buffer enabling one to maintain alignment with personal values. Moreover, strategies that help recognize and navigate intense social pressures—whether through withdrawal from compromising situations or mental reframing—are crucial for preserving moral agency.
In conclusion, this contemporary inquiry into obedience dynamics reaffirms the timeless insights of Milgram’s ground-breaking work while advancing our understanding through a modern psychological perspective. It challenges the comforting illusion of moral exceptionalism and lays bare the subtle yet potent forces shaping human compliance. As societies confront authoritarianism and ethical dilemmas in an ever-complex world, such research provides a critical foundation for fostering both individual and collective integrity.
Subject of Research: Social psychology study on self-perception, obedience to authority, and the better-than-average effect in the context of the Milgram shock experiment.
Article Title: Milgram shock-study imaginal replication: how far do you think you would go?
News Publication Date: 29-May-2025
Web References:
References:
Mazzocco, P. J., Reitler, K., Little, L., Korte, J., Ridgill, M., & Stalnaker, X. (2025). Milgram shock-study imaginal replication: how far do you think you would go? Current Psychology.
Keywords:
Social psychology, social influence, obedience, authority, better-than-average effect, personality traits, conscientiousness, Milgram experiment, social pressure, ethical behavior, psychological resilience