Monday, August 18, 2025
Science
No Result
View All Result
  • Login
  • HOME
  • SCIENCE NEWS
  • CONTACT US
  • HOME
  • SCIENCE NEWS
  • CONTACT US
No Result
View All Result
Scienmag
No Result
View All Result
Home Science News Biology

Why do researchers often prefer safe over risky projects? Explaining risk aversion in science

August 15, 2024
in Biology
Reading Time: 3 mins read
0
66
SHARES
598
VIEWS
Share on FacebookShare on Twitter
ADVERTISEMENT
ADVERTISEMENT

A mathematical framework that builds on the economic theory of hidden-action models provides insight into how the unobservable nature of effort and risk shapes investigators’ research strategies and the incentive structures within which they work, according to a study published August 15th in the open-access journal PLOS Biology by Kevin Gross from North Carolina State University, U.S., and Carl Bergstrom from the University of Washington, U.S. 

A mathematical framework that builds on the economic theory of hidden-action models provides insight into how the unobservable nature of effort and risk shapes investigators’ research strategies and the incentive structures within which they work, according to a study published August 15th in the open-access journal PLOS Biology by Kevin Gross from North Carolina State University, U.S., and Carl Bergstrom from the University of Washington, U.S. 

Scientific research requires taking risks, as the most cautious approaches are unlikely to lead to the most rapid progress. Yet much funded scientific research plays it safe and funding agencies bemoan the difficulty of attracting high-risk, high-return research projects. Gross and Bergstrom adapted an economic contracting model to explore how the unobservability of risk and effort discourages risky research.

The model considers a hidden-action problem, in which the scientific community must reward discoveries in a way that encourages effort and risk-taking while simultaneously protecting researchers’ livelihoods against the unpredictability of scientific outcomes. Its challenge when doing so is that incentives to motivate effort clash with incentives to motivate risk-taking, because a failed project may be evidence of a risky undertaking but could also be the result of simple sloth. As a result, the incentives that are needed to encourage effort do actively discourage risk-taking.

Scientists respond by working on safe projects that generate evidence of effort but that don’t move science forward as rapidly as riskier projects would. A social planner who prizes scientific productivity above researchers’ well-being could remedy the problem by rewarding major discoveries richly enough to induce high-risk research, but in doing so would expose scientists to a degree of livelihood risk that ultimately leaves them worse off. Because the scientific community is approximately self-governing and constructs its own reward schedule, the incentives that researchers are willing to impose on themselves are inadequate to motivate the scientific risks that would best expedite scientific progress.

In deciding how to reward discoveries, the scientific community must contend with the fact that reward schemes that motivate effort inherently discourage scientific risk-taking, and vice versa. Because the community must motivate both effort and scientific risk-taking, and because effort is costly to investigators, the community inevitably establishes a tradition that encourages more conservative science than would be optimal for maximizing scientific progress, even when risky research is no more onerous than safer lines of inquiry.

The authors add, “Commentators regularly bemoan the dearth of high-risk, high-return research in science and suppose that this state of affairs is evidence of institutional or personal failings.  We argue here that this is not the case; instead, scientists who don’t want to gamble with their careers will inevitably choose projects that are safer than scientific funders would prefer.”

#####

In your coverage, please use this URL to provide access to the freely available paper in PLOS Biology:

Citation: Gross K, Bergstrom CT (2024) Rationalizing risk aversion in science: Why incentives to work hard clash with incentives to take risks. PLoS Biol 22(8): e3002750.

Author Countries: United States

Funding: This work was supported by National Science Foundation awards SMA 19-52343 and SMA 19-52069 to KG and CTB, respectively, and by Templeton World Charity Foundation grant 20650 to CTB. No funder played any role in the study design, analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.



Journal

PLoS Biology

DOI

10.1371/journal.pbio.3002750

Method of Research

Computational simulation/modeling

Subject of Research

Not applicable

COI Statement

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Share26Tweet17
Previous Post

‘Legos for wireless research’: Rice-led university consortium tackles the future of wireless with $4.5M NSF award

Next Post

Chicxulub impactor was a carbonaceous-type asteroid from beyond Jupiter

Related Posts

Biology

The Hidden Costs of Early Puberty and Childbirth

August 18, 2025
blank
Biology

Vacuolar Receptors Drive Plant Immunity via Autophagy

August 18, 2025
blank
Biology

When Rattlesnakes Mate with Their Cousins: New Insights into Genetic Relationships

August 18, 2025
blank
Biology

Decoding Glucose Congestion in Type 2 Diabetes

August 18, 2025
blank
Biology

Microbial Community Mimics Fine Chocolate Fermentation Traits

August 18, 2025
blank
Biology

In the Blink of an Eye: How River Noise Influences the Dipper’s Silent Signals

August 18, 2025
Next Post

Chicxulub impactor was a carbonaceous-type asteroid from beyond Jupiter

  • Mothers who receive childcare support from maternal grandparents show more parental warmth, finds NTU Singapore study

    Mothers who receive childcare support from maternal grandparents show more parental warmth, finds NTU Singapore study

    27535 shares
    Share 11011 Tweet 6882
  • University of Seville Breaks 120-Year-Old Mystery, Revises a Key Einstein Concept

    949 shares
    Share 380 Tweet 237
  • Bee body mass, pathogens and local climate influence heat tolerance

    641 shares
    Share 256 Tweet 160
  • Researchers record first-ever images and data of a shark experiencing a boat strike

    507 shares
    Share 203 Tweet 127
  • Warm seawater speeding up melting of ‘Doomsday Glacier,’ scientists warn

    311 shares
    Share 124 Tweet 78
Science

Embark on a thrilling journey of discovery with Scienmag.com—your ultimate source for cutting-edge breakthroughs. Immerse yourself in a world where curiosity knows no limits and tomorrow’s possibilities become today’s reality!

RECENT NEWS

  • Liquid Phase Separation Patterns Predict Pediatric AML Outcomes
  • Silver-Doped Zirconium Copper Oxide Detects Dihydroxybenzene Isomers
  • Researchers Unveil the Mechanisms Behind Protein Language Models
  • Aircraft Toilets May Help Halt the Spread of Global Superbugs

Categories

  • Agriculture
  • Anthropology
  • Archaeology
  • Athmospheric
  • Biology
  • Bussines
  • Cancer
  • Chemistry
  • Climate
  • Earth Science
  • Marine
  • Mathematics
  • Medicine
  • Pediatry
  • Policy
  • Psychology & Psychiatry
  • Science Education
  • Social Science
  • Space
  • Technology and Engineering

Subscribe to Blog via Email

Enter your email address to subscribe to this blog and receive notifications of new posts by email.

Join 4,859 other subscribers

© 2025 Scienmag - Science Magazine

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In
No Result
View All Result
  • HOME
  • SCIENCE NEWS
  • CONTACT US

© 2025 Scienmag - Science Magazine

Discover more from Science

Subscribe now to keep reading and get access to the full archive.

Continue reading