Monday, August 18, 2025
Science
No Result
View All Result
  • Login
  • HOME
  • SCIENCE NEWS
  • CONTACT US
  • HOME
  • SCIENCE NEWS
  • CONTACT US
No Result
View All Result
Scienmag
No Result
View All Result
Home Science News Social Science

What is the neural mechanism behind helping someone at your own cost?

June 12, 2024
in Social Science
Reading Time: 4 mins read
0
65
SHARES
593
VIEWS
Share on FacebookShare on Twitter
ADVERTISEMENT
ADVERTISEMENT

Using a unique setup, researchers from the Social Brain Lab at the Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience have researched the neural mechanism behind a universal dilemma: deciding whether to help someone else even when it involves a personal sacrifice.

Using a unique setup, researchers from the Social Brain Lab at the Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience have researched the neural mechanism behind a universal dilemma: deciding whether to help someone else even when it involves a personal sacrifice.

We often have opportunities to give up something we care about to help others. What brain mechanisms help us make those decisions, and why do some people help more than others?

Over the years, philosophers and scientists have suggested that the extent to which a person empathizes with the distress of others influences their willingness to help.

To explore this hypothesis experimentally, Kalliopi Ioumpa and Selene Gallo, under the supervision of Christian Keysers and Valeria Gazzola, investigated pro-social behaviour—voluntary behaviour intended to benefit others—in self-reported mirror-pain synaesthetes.  Mirror-pain synaesthetes report feeling pain on their own body whenever they’re witnessing pain on someone else. If sharing the pain of others is indeed a key motivator to help others, these individuals should be particularly generous.

“To better understand why some people help more than others, in the past, our lab had performed experiments with participants that are at the lower extreme of empathy, including individuals with psychopathy. This time, we wanted to look at the opposite extreme: people that feel the pain of others so much more, that it feels like the pain is on their own body” explained Christian Keysers. “In the literature, there was some notion that these participants feel such a localized pain when witnessing that of others, because their brain activated somatosensory cortices – regions normally only involved in the sense of touch on your own body. Would that influence their donations?”.

The conflict scenario

Studying a realistic conflict in a controlled lab-setting requires a creative approach in which participants feel that their decisions have real consequences both for themselves and for someone else. “If you just ask people whether they are generous or not, most of them like to say yes” Christian says with a smile. “To reveal real differences between people, you have to create a situation in which the distress of others feels real, and where helping has a real cost for participants” Valeria emphasizes.

In their study, they created such a conflict by making participants believe that they were participating in the study together with another participant. In reality, the other participant was an actor, which kept each experiment consistent.

In the experiment the two ‘participants’ were allocated two different roles: one, the actor, who was filmed while receiving painful stimulations on their hand; the other, the participant, who received real money and could donate some of this money to alleviate some of the other participant’s pain. “All participants believed that the other was really receiving the painful stimulation on their hand in ways that depended on their donations” Kalliopi Ioumpa explains. “In reality we pre-recorded the actor receiving these painful stimulations ahead of time, so that all participants saw the exact same stimuli”.

Because participants made their decisions in the fMRI scanner, the researchers could analyse the brain areas related to their decisions, and how they differed between those that feel pain on their own body when they see the pain of others, and those that do not. Kalliopi Ioumpa: “We wanted to explore how much different participants would help and what the underlying neural mechanisms would be.”

Mirror-pain synaesthetes and prosocial behaviour

Compared to control participants, the self-reported mirror-pain synaesthetes donated significantly more money to alleviate the other participant’s pain – giving credence to the belief that the more you feel the pain of others, the more you are willing to give up to help them.

As expected, the researchers also found significant differences between the two groups when looking at the brain mechanism “self-reported mirror pain synaesthetes activated their secondary somatosensory cortex more. This brain area is involved in sensory experiences such as touch or pain”, Kalliopi Ioumpa explains. “So while most of us just feel generally uncomfortable when we see others in pain, mirror touch synaesthetes have a higher activation in their somatosensory cortex, as if they are experiencing pain in the corresponding body parts themselves, and they are more motivated to help” adds Christian.

Towards more prosocial behaviour

After each experiment, participants are told the truth about the other participant being an actor, and they receive the amount of money they chose to keep.

Kalliopi Ioumpa: “We know that someone’s level of empathy can change. People can learn to enhance it but also to shut it down. Consider doctors for example, they tend to become less empathetic over the years because they need to block their empathy”. Valeria Gazzola: “and as I explain in my TEDx talk, we can all modulate how empathic we want to be in a particular situation. Empathy is not a fixed trait, it’s something we have control over, and that, as we show here, can influence how prosocial we decide to be”.

“This study helps us understand prosocial behaviour better as well, as its links to empathy. We show that a group of highly empathic individuals, that somatically feels the pain of others, also demonstrates enhanced pro-social behaviour”, Kalliopi Ioumpa explains. The researchers hope that their work can inspire interventions to enhance empathy and prosocial behaviour in the future.

Source: Nature

 

Correspondence:

Kalliopi Ioumpa, ioumpa.k@gmail.com

Valeria Gazzola, v.gazzola@nin.knaw.nl



Journal

Scientific Reports

DOI

10.1038/s41598-024-62422-3

Share26Tweet16
Previous Post

AI better detects prostate cancer on MRI than radiologists

Next Post

USC EdTech Accelerator collaborates with Intel

Related Posts

Social Science

Exploring Counterfactual Learning on Graphs: A Comprehensive Survey

August 18, 2025
blank
Social Science

Unraveling Resilience: Methods in Longitudinal Studies

August 18, 2025
blank
Social Science

Origin’s Impact on Migrant Mortality in Spain

August 18, 2025
blank
Social Science

Bridging Social and Personal Views on Sexual Harassment

August 18, 2025
blank
Social Science

Corpus Study Reveals Arabic Translations of ‘Necessary’

August 18, 2025
blank
Social Science

Why Students Choose Grammarly: Insights and Influences

August 18, 2025
Next Post

USC EdTech Accelerator collaborates with Intel

  • Mothers who receive childcare support from maternal grandparents show more parental warmth, finds NTU Singapore study

    Mothers who receive childcare support from maternal grandparents show more parental warmth, finds NTU Singapore study

    27535 shares
    Share 11011 Tweet 6882
  • University of Seville Breaks 120-Year-Old Mystery, Revises a Key Einstein Concept

    949 shares
    Share 380 Tweet 237
  • Bee body mass, pathogens and local climate influence heat tolerance

    641 shares
    Share 256 Tweet 160
  • Researchers record first-ever images and data of a shark experiencing a boat strike

    507 shares
    Share 203 Tweet 127
  • Warm seawater speeding up melting of ‘Doomsday Glacier,’ scientists warn

    311 shares
    Share 124 Tweet 78
Science

Embark on a thrilling journey of discovery with Scienmag.com—your ultimate source for cutting-edge breakthroughs. Immerse yourself in a world where curiosity knows no limits and tomorrow’s possibilities become today’s reality!

RECENT NEWS

  • Wired Brain: New Encoding-Decoding Neural Communication Insights
  • How Job Stress Affects Police Well-Being
  • DENND1A Drives Testosterone in Polycystic Ovary Syndrome
  • Sub-Arc Mantle Oxidized Since Neoproterozoic Era

Categories

  • Agriculture
  • Anthropology
  • Archaeology
  • Athmospheric
  • Biology
  • Bussines
  • Cancer
  • Chemistry
  • Climate
  • Earth Science
  • Marine
  • Mathematics
  • Medicine
  • Pediatry
  • Policy
  • Psychology & Psychiatry
  • Science Education
  • Social Science
  • Space
  • Technology and Engineering

Subscribe to Blog via Email

Enter your email address to subscribe to this blog and receive notifications of new posts by email.

Join 4,859 other subscribers

© 2025 Scienmag - Science Magazine

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In
No Result
View All Result
  • HOME
  • SCIENCE NEWS
  • CONTACT US

© 2025 Scienmag - Science Magazine

Discover more from Science

Subscribe now to keep reading and get access to the full archive.

Continue reading